A Descriptive Analysis of Kinematic and Electromyographic Relationships of the Core During Forward Stepping in Beginning and Expert Dancers
Steven J. Chatfield, Ph.D., Donna H. Krasnow, M.S., Amanda Herman, and Glenna Blessing, M.F.A.
Journal of Dance Medicine & Science, Volume 11, Number 3, 2007.
Abstract
While electromyographic (EMG) and kinematic
data in dance are accumulating, to
date these data have raised more questions
than they have answered. The purpose
of this study was to introduce ensemble
averaging into this body of literature as
a way of dealing with the high levels of
within-subject and between-subject variability
that have been previously reported.
This study also introduces analysis during
a forward weight shift, an analysis currently
absent from the literature. Three collegiate
novices (18.7 ± 0.6 years of age) and three
expert dancers (27.7 ± 5.5 years of age)
were studied in-depth. EMG data were collected
continuously at 600 Hz for analysis
of onset of activity for abdominal and erector
spinae muscles. Kinematic data were
collected continuously at 120 Hz from
markers on the acromion and the greater
trochanter for analysis of the verticality of
the trunk. Data were collected continuously
for over 4 seconds to include: baseline
data prior to movement on a right legged
balance, data for movement into plié fondu
on the right leg, data for a forward step to
the left leg, and baseline data at resolution
on a left legged balance. For analysis, data
were synchronized by time using onset of
vertical ground reaction forces recorded by
a force plate under the initial stance leg.
All participants were tested on two separate
days to assess day-to-day variability.
Fifteen trials were collected on each day
for each individual. Ensemble averaging
of continuously recorded data was used
to create line graphs for visual inspection,
first to compare day-to-day congruence
for each individual, next to assess within
group variability, and finally to compare
composite graphs between groups. Day-to-day variations for each individual were
minimal. Differences were seen between
members of the Beginner group but not the
Expert group. Between group comparisons
revealed the following differences: Experts
appeared to use an anterior core support
strategy while Beginners appeared to use a
posterior core support strategy, Experts displayed
less EMG and kinematic variability
than Beginners, and Experts maintained a
more vertical posture throughout. Surprisingly,
even though Experts were more vertical,
they demonstrated the same amount
of overall anterior-posterior sway as the
Beginners. This finding leads to discussion
of the dynamic nature of neuromuscular
coordination patterns in maintenance of
verticality. Issues surrounding the inability
of statistically constructed models of human kinematic data to accurately represent
individuals in groups are also discussed.
Finally, applications of these findings to
teaching and learning are offered.
The understanding and investigation
of muscular activity and
skeletal alignment in dancers
has been a subject of interest for educators
and researchers for centuries, going
back to Weaver in 17211 and Blasis.[2] In
the last century, more extensive descriptions
of alignment and body use can be
found in the literature.[3,4] However, it is
only in the last 25 years that measurement
tools such as electromyographic
and kinematic analysis have been used
in dance science research. Several studies
have begun to shed light on muscle
action and skeletal alignment during
dance-specific movement.[5-15]
In 1992 Mouchnino and colleagues11
compared experienced modern dancers
to subjects with no previous athletic or
dance training. The movement task was
a well-known dance movement, dégagé
à la seconde to 45° from turned out first
position. All subjects performed 4 trials
with each leg in a random sequence.
In paradigm 1, the subjects were asked
to do the movement as fast as possible,
and maintain the resulting balance for a
few seconds. In paradigm 2, they were
asked additionally to keep the trunk
vertical during the task. The movement
was defined in two phases, the ballistic
phase in which the gesture leg initiated
the dégagé, and the adjustment phase
in which the body found a new balance
on the supporting leg. Mouchnino and colleagues[11] noted four differences. First,
dancers arrived at the final balance at the
end of the ballistic phase, and thus had a
very short adjustment phase, non-dancers
had two distinct phases, including a
long adjustment phase. Second, dancers
had anticipatory responses to the dégagé,
whereas the non-dancers had compensatory
responses, that is, in the dancers,
the muscles of the supporting leg and
trunk fired prior to the muscles of the
gesture leg. Third, dancers minimized
the center of gravity displacement to the
supporting leg, whereas the non-dancers
showed a larger displacement of the
center of gravity. And fourth, the subjects
demonstrated two distinct movement
strategies. The dancers demonstrated a
translation strategy, in which the hips
remained level and the trunk shifted as
one unit over to the new support base.
The non-dancers demonstrated an inclination
strategy in which the hips and
head tilted to accommodate the weight
shift to the new support base and which
resulted in a bending of the trunk to keep
the head vertical. They note that in the
translation strategy, only two joints (the
ankle and hip) need to be regulated, and
their adjustments are equal and opposite,
whereas in the inclination strategy, there
are multiple joint adjustments, making
the resulting action less predictable and
consistent. Mouchnino and colleagues11
suggested that these differences account
for much less variability in individual
dancer’s trials and may be the result of
long years of training.
Two more recent studies conducted
electromyographic analysis of professional
ballet and modern dancers,
examining muscle use in stance, demiplié,
and grand plié. In a 1994 study
by Trepman and coworkers,[13] dancers
performed five repetitions of a 6-second
demi-plié, which was analyzed in
two phases, the lowering phase and
the rising phase. In a 1998 study by
Trepman and associates,[14] dancers performed
five repetitions of an 8-second
grand plié, which was analyzed in four
subdivided phases: early lowering phase
(start to heel-off), late lowering phase
(heel-off to mid-cycle), early rising
phase (mid-cycle to heel-on), and late
rising phase (heel-on to end). These
studies found that as groups, ballet
and modern dancers showed different
patterns of muscle use. Furthermore,
variability was seen both within each
subject as well as between subjects of
the same group. Interestingly, it was
found that the early lowering phase
of the grand plié (often thought to be
the same as the lowering phase of a
demi-plié) was performed differently
during demi-plié and grand plié. The
grand plié engaged more muscles (adductors,
hamstrings, quadriceps, and
tibialis anterior) in the early phase. The
demi-plié was primarily quadriceps
controlled. These findings yield indications
that:
1. movements that appeared
similar in skeletal alignment employed
varying neuromuscular strategies, and
2. within any given strategy, there was
regularly occurring variability across
trials.
Knowing that the relationship of
kinematic and electromyographic data
in dance varies between individuals
of similar expertise and, more importantly,
even varies from trial to trial
within the performances of the same
individual, the intent of the current
study was to employ both within and
between subject analysis. In doing so,
each individual’s data were preserved
for analysis. This differs from study
designs that rely solely on group analysis.
In group analysis, individual data
are sacrificed into anonymity through
group averaging and other statistical
procedures.
To date, all of the studies conducted
have been on dancers who are either
standing still or moving without traveling
through space, as in stepping. The
transfer of weight from one foot to the
other across an open stance is a major
component of dance that has not yet
been analyzed using kinematics and
electromyography. The purpose of this
study was to investigate the relationship
between select muscle recruitment
and skeletal alignment (verticality)
while performing dance movements
involving an initial one-legged (right)
balance, a forward shift of weight
through space, and a resultant onelegged
(left) balance at resolution. We
expected to find that when compared
to beginning dance students, expert
dancers performing this movement
would demonstrate the following: 1.
maintenance of a more vertical posture,
and 2. more frequent abdominal EMG
responses.
Methods
The subjects in this study were three
expert dancers with mixed expertise
in ballet and modern dance who were
teaching and performing in a professional
capacity, and three dance students
with beginning level exposure to ballet
and modern dance in a university dance
program who were matched by height
and weight with the expert dancers.
Table 1 provides subject descriptions.
Beginners averaged 3 hours of technique
class weekly with no regularly scheduled
rehearsals and no other regular physical
training. Experts averaged 6 hours
weekly of advanced technique class and
12 hours weekly of regularly scheduled
rehearsals. Experts were not involved
in any other regular physical training.
Human Subjects Approval was received
and all participants signed Informed
Consent forms.
Testing consisted of a single step
from the right foot to the left foot.
Each subject’s leg was measured from
the greater trochanter to the lateral
malleolus and this distance was marked
on the floor. This defined the distance
of the forward step they were asked to
perform as relative to each participant’s
stature (see “leg/step length” in Table
1). The movement was demonstrated
for the subjects and scripted instructions
clarified that the movement was
to be done while maintaining vertical
alignment. In each trial, the subject did
two movements in 1 second timed to
an audible metronome set at 120 beats
per minute. These two movements were
sandwiched by baseline data at the start
and baseline data at resolution. At the
start, prior to moving, each subject stood
ready on a one-legged balance (right leg)
to establish at least 1 second of baseline
data. On the first count after being cued
in, the subject performed a parallel plié
on the supporting right leg while reaching
the gesture (left) leg forward. On the
second count, the subject shifted weight
forward (the distance of their trochanter
to lateral malleolus measurement) onto a
straight left leg, drawing the right leg to
a low parallel gesture. The subject then
maintained balance on the second leg to
record baseline data at resolution. This created 4 phases to the test movement
that will be referred to throughout this
report:
Phase 1. baseline-at-start,
Phase 2. plié fondu,
Phase 3. shift of weight, and
Phase 4. baseline at resolution.
Four seconds of continuous kinematic
and electromyographic data were
collected beginning prior to the movement
and continuing into the resolution
phase. A force plate was used under the
initial stance leg to record vertical ground
reaction forces. For analysis, the onset of
vertical ground reaction forces was used
to synchronize all data by time. Baseline
data at the start provided information
against which phase changes associated
with the movements could be seen.
For the kinematics, reflective markers
were placed on the acromion of the
scapula and the greater trochanter of the
femur (acromio-trochanter segment).
Kinematic data were collected using a
4 camera PEAK System (Peak Performance
Inc., Colorado, USA). Space was
calibrated to within a maximum of 2
mm error for a 1 m long wand. Threedimensional
data were collected at 120
Hz. The 3-D position data were low-pass
filtered between 4-8 Hz using a fourth
order dual-pass Butterworth filter and
recorded at 400 Hz.
Electromyographic data were collected
using bipolar surface electrodes
(DE-02, Delsys, MA, USA). EMG
signals were on-line preamplified (x
7000), analog filtered (20-7000 Hz),
and digitally converted at 600 Hz using
the Associated Measurement Laboratory
data acquisition system (AMLAB Inc.,
Sydney, Australia). For analysis, EMG
signals were full-wave rectified, low-pass
filtered at 6 Hz using a fourth order dualpass
Butterworth filter, and recorded
at 600 Hz. Abdominal electrodes were
placed bilaterally in the center of a triangle
formed by the inguinal ligament,
a line from anterior superior iliac spine
to umbilicus, and the midline.16 Erector
spinae electrodes were centered over the
belly of the muscle at the level of the
second lumbar vertebra.
Each subject was tested on two separate
days to assess day-to-day variability.
Testing continued each day until data for
15 successful trials were collected. Data
were collected in the motor control lab
in the middle or late day. No systematic
warm-up or any other preparation was
given to participants. Testing sessions
typically lasted approximately 90 minutes.
During the initial 60 minutes,
subjects were first introduced to the lab
team and briefed on the testing protocol.
Then the distance between their greater
trochanter and lateral malleolus of their
right leg was measured and footprints
were manufactured and placed on the
force plate and resolution stance space
to reflect this distance. Next, they were
outfitted with reflective markers for
kinematic data collection and electrodes
for EMG data collection. After outfitting
was completed, the actual movement
testing began and took approximately
30 minutes.
Analysis
Line graphs were created for visual
inspection. These graphs contained
continuously collected kinematic and
EMG data starting with pre-movement
baseline data and including data over
the full time course of the movement
as well as baseline data at resolution. All
data were ensemble averaged to create
one graphic representation of multiple
trials. Ensemble averaging is the name
given to the average patterns that can
be generated for repeated trials of any
continuously recorded variable. An
ensemble average is generated by calculating
the mean and standard deviation
of a variable for every data acquisition
interval for repeated trails of the same
activity. For use in ensemble averaging,
data need to be continuous values
collected over the entire duration of
the sample activity. Means and SD’s
for each data acquisition interval are
generated by ensemble averaging and
plotted on line graphs for the entire
time period of the sample activity.
For this analysis, graphs for each
parameter were constructed using the
ensemble average of 15 trials for each
subject for each day. After visual inspection
of the graphic waveforms ruled out
day-to-day differences for each subject,
graphs for each parameter were made
that combined results for day 1 and
day 2. These graphs displayed ensemble
data for all 30 trials of each subject and
were used to assess similarities and differences
between participants within
each group. Finally, ensemble graphs
representing group performances were
made for each parameter using all 90
trials for the beginners and all 90 trials
for the experts. In all instances the
ensemble averages were represented
by the middle line of three lines on
each graph. The ensemble average was
bordered above and below by lines representing
plus and minus one standard deviation.
The term “sway” was used to describe
the change in degrees of verticality
occurring at the intersection
of a vertical line and the line formed
by the acromio-trochanter segment.
Graphs for sway and EMG responses
were aligned for visual inspection in a
time-synchronized format, using the
onset of vertical ground reaction forces
to establish a relative zero across the
trials.
Kinematic Results and Discussion
In describing sway, two factors were
examined: 1. acromio-trochanter
position relative to vertical at any
point in time, and 2. the shape of the
acromio-trochanter segment waveform
over time. Figure 1 displays ensemble
data representing 90 trials for each
group and for each parameter. Vertical
ground reaction forces were used to
align movement onset at 500 msec for
each parameter in Figure 1. These 500
msec of data pre-movement represent
the baseline against which phasic onsets
of waveforms were assessed.
Anterior-posterior sway of the acromio-trochanter segment (AP sway) is
represented in the top graph of Figure
1 for each group. Mean baseline values
for the initial 500 msec of these waveforms
are -9.75° ±.1.25° for Beginners
and -4.50° ± 0.75° for Experts. In other
words, the “ready” posture for Beginners
incorporated a 9.75° posterior tilt
for the acromio-trochanter segment
relative to vertical. The Experts readied
themselves with a 4.50° posterior tilt.
Over time, ensemble data for both
Beginners and Experts produce a
similar waveform for AP sway. Upward
deflections of these waveforms indicate
anterior sway and downward deflections
indicate posterior sway (Table
2). During the plié fondu, sway is
anterior. The maximum value for this
anterior sway relative to the starting
baseline is +1.5° for Beginners and
+4.0° for Experts (Table 3). During
the shift of weight, sway is posterior.
The minimum, or most posterior,
values relative to the starting baseline
are -3.75° for Beginners and -1.75° for
Experts. Another way to consider these
data is that the Beginners’ sway was
more than twice as large in its posterior
than its anterior dimension, while the
converse was true for the Experts (i.e.,
their anterior sway was more than twice
as large as their posterior sway).
Baseline at resolution was -10.25° ±
2.50° for Beginners and -1.25° ± 1.00°
for Experts. That is to say, at resolution,
acromio-trochanter segments were
10.25° posterior to vertical for Beginners,
and 1.25° posterior to vertical for
Experts. These values at resolution indicate
that Beginners ended with 0.50°
more posterior tilt than they started
with while Experts ended with 3.25°
more anterior tilt than they started
with. The absolute range of sway was
calculated as the maximum minus the
minimum values of AP sway. Surprisingly,
this 5.25° range was the same for
both groups.
In summary, a description of the Beginners’
AP sway ensemble data is that
they readied themselves with a 9.75°
posterior tilt from vertical, swayed 1.5°
in an anterior direction during the first
phase of the movement, swayed 5.25°
posterior during the second phase of
the movement, and resolved with a
posterior tilt 10.25° behind vertical. A
descriptive movement summary for the
Experts is that they readied themselves
with a 4.50° posterior tilt from vertical,
swayed 4.0° in an anterior direction
during the first phase of the movement,
swayed 5.25° posterior during the
second phase of the movement, and resolved
with a posterior tilt 1.25° behind
vertical. Compared to the Beginners,
the Experts appeared to be different
in that they started closer to vertical,
remained more vertical throughout the
movement, finished closer to vertical,
and actually improved toward vertical at resolution compared to their starting
position.
In the study by Mouchnino and
colleagues,[11] it was suggested that
dancers employ optimal strategies for
shift of weight that reduce kinematic
variability and compensatory activity
and emphasize verticality. As described
above, the current study appears to
support those findings. In addition,
Mouchnino and colleagues found
that dancers demonstrated reduced
kinematic variability during repeated
performance of the same movement.
The current study supports these findings.
In the current study, the reduced
variability for the Experts can be seen
in the relative nearness of the lines representing
±1 standard deviation to the
mean AP sway in Figure 1 as compared
to the spread of the SD lines for the
Beginners. As can be seen in Table 2,
SD values for Beginners during baseline-at-start, maximum, minimum,
and baseline-at-resolution range from
164% to 250% of comparable values
for the Experts.
However, Beginners and Experts
were not different in the overall range
of AP sway that they demonstrated.
Both groups swayed 5.25° as calculated
by subtracting the maximum and minimum
values of their AP sway characteristics.
While the Experts swayed just as
much as the Beginners throughout the
overall time course of the movements,
they did so around an axis that was
closer to vertical throughout.
EMG Results and Discussion
Visual analysis of EMG findings focused
on the onsets of phasic bursts.
From this analysis the frequency of
phasic EMG responses was assessed
and select temporal characteristics of
the wave forms were described (i.e.,
the timing of bursts relative to one
another and to the time course of the
movement).
Both Beginners and Experts demonstrated
100% response rates for abdominal and erector spinae EMG
bursts. In other words, both Beginners
and Experts displayed robust abdominal and erector spinae muscle activation
responses during every trial. Examination
of the Beginners’ ensemble
EMG data (Fig. 1) reveals that the right
abdominal EMG (R Abd EMG) ensemble
response shows a double burst
pattern with the first, lower amplitude
spike coincident with the anterior sway
during plié fondue. The second, higher
amplitude R Abd EMG burst occurs
later, in synchrony with the first burst
of the left abdominal EMG response
(L Abd EMG). These aligned R and L
Abd EMG bursts occur during the final
phases of the posterior sway, after toe
off from the thrusting leg and during
deceleration of forward momentum
on the new support leg, just before
the beginning of the anterior sway
leading to the resolution baseline. The
L Abd EMG then exhibits a pattern of
multiple bursts during resolution.
For the Experts, R Abd EMG and L
Abd EMG responses show fairly unified
single bursts that appear to be relatively
synchronous with one another
and occur during the forward weight
shift, beginning midway through the
posterior sway. Both the R and L Abd
EMG responses for the Experts then
have a minor second burst on the
down slope of the first that coincides
with the transition into, and the initial
phases of, the anterior sway that leads
to movement resolution as stability
is regained after the forward step is
completed.
These minor second bursts for the
Experts are not as pronounced as the
double burst for the Beginners’ R Abd EMG or the multiple bursts spread
across the resolution baseline for the
Beginners’ L Abd EMG. In comparison
with the Beginners, the Experts’ overall
Abd EMG activity appears to be more
synchronized bilaterally and more optimally
sequenced in time to anticipate
and transition into recovery from the
posterior sway phase of the forward
stepping movement.
A converse pattern of unitary versus
multiple bursts exists for the groups for
erector spinae EMG responses (R ES
EMG and L ES EMG). For the Beginners,
R and L ES EMG bursts appear
synchronous and demonstrate a single
focused burst during the weight shift,
before their aligned R and L Abd EMG
bursts, as though a posterior muscular
synergy was the focus of the neuromotor
strategy and antagonistic anterior
responses acted to modulate the posterior
response. The Beginners’ ES EMG
responses preceded their Abd EMG responses
by approximately 50 msec. Their
ES EMG bursts occurred during the
early phase of the posterior sway. Their
collected Abd EMG responses occurred
at the end of the weight shift as posterior
sway reversed and the anterior sway
leading to resolution was beginning. In
other words, their collected Abd EMG
bursts began during the development
of the final anterior sway, which arrived
at a relatively stable baseline of 10.50°
posterior acromio-trochanter segment
deviation from vertical during movement
resolution on a one-legged balance.
To the naked eye, this EMG pattern was
associated with a movement that looked
like a backward bowing during weight
shift, followed by an impulse forward,
which persisted as a jerky resolution
preventing them from falling backward
out of the final balance.
This response is plausibly explained as
a compensatory hip strategy as described
by Horak and Nashner.[17] The hip strategy
is a compensatory reaction to loss
of balance from posterior sway generated
by a forward thrust. The ES EMG
responses could be part of a synergy
of posterior muscles actively backward
bending the body to form a bow from
head to foot that sends the body’s overall
center of gravity forward to counterbalance
the posterior displacement of the
head and shoulders. The Abd EMG
responses could be part of a synergy of
anterior muscles to reduce the bowing of
the body and bring the center of gravity
back on top of the stance to resolve the
backward bowing.
By contrast, the Experts’ ES EMG
responses occurred approximately 150
msec after their Abd EMG responses.
Their collected Abd EMG responses occurred
in the midst of the posterior sway
coinciding with the forward weight shift.
Their ES EMG bursts began during the
development of the anterior sway which
arrived at development of a relatively
stable baseline during resolution with
1.25° posterior acromio-trochanter segment
deviation from vertical.
Theoretically, the Experts’ dynamic
series of EMG responses seems to describe
an optimal neuromotor strategy
in which Abd EMG responses during
the posterior sway are sequenced in time
to anticipate the anterior sway that leads
to resolution. This could be part of an
anterior muscular synergy that helps
stabilize the acromio-trochanter segment
near vertical on top of a straight standing
leg. The ES EMG indicates muscle
activity during the anterior sway as part
of a posterior muscle synergy to decelerate
the forward sway of the acromiotrochanter
segment to reach movement
resolution and reinforce verticality of
the acromio-trochanter segment on a
straight standing leg with fully extended
hip and knee joints.
Summary Discussion
We expected to find that when compared
to beginning dance students,
expert dancers performing a dance
movement incorporating a forward
weight shift would demonstrate: 1.
maintenance of a more vertical posture
and 2. more frequent abdominal EMG
responses. On the first point, the Experts
did demonstrate a more vertical
preparation, execution, and resolution
of the movement. However, surprising
to us in this regard was that their overall
range of sway (5.25°) was the same range
of sway seen in the Beginners’ ensemble
results. We found this surprising because,
in retrospect, we realized that we had
assumed maintaining a more vertical
acromio-trochanter segment would be
correlated with reduced sway overall.
Apparently, this is not the case.
While this magnitude of dynamic AP
sway for the experts was unexpected, it is
interesting to theorize how this dynamic
alignment might be a desirable strategy
for dancers. First it should be noted that
in real time this magnitude of AP sway
is not noticeable to the naked eye. To
live observers, and when viewing video
footage of their performances in real
time, the Experts “appeared” to maintain
their verticality throughout the movement.
The dynamic nature of this sway
phenomenon runs counter to concepts
of stiffening joints to “hold” postures
during movement. Rather, a dynamic
conceptualization of responsive posture
during movement suggests active,
ongoing kinematic and neuromuscular
relationships between automatic core
support mechanisms, training adaptations,
and voluntary intentions.[18] As
Luttgens and Hamilton state, “posture
influences all we do and…it is not a
static but a dynamic configuration.”19
It is interesting to note that, based
on their acromio-trochanter segment
sway patterns over time, the ensemble
data for both Beginners and Experts in
this study appear to display kinematic
similarities to normal gait patterns described
by Winter.[20] The ensemble
results in this study describe forward
tilting step initiations with backward
tilting decelerations of forward thrust
to arrest forward stepping momentum
after weight shift and transition to a final
forward tilting adjustment to establish
erect posture on a straight standing leg
at movement resolution. However, later
in this discussion, the representativeness
of the Beginners’ ensemble group model
will be examined and questioned. It
seems that all of the Experts do indeed
display the characteristics that Winter
describes as normal while 2 of the 3 Beginners
do not. This could be explained
by the fact that the movement used in
this study was not a “normal gait” pattern.
It involved stylized arm, leg, and
torso use. While the Experts appear to
use a “normal” gait pattern in terms of
acromio-trochanter segment sway, even
during this stylized movement, perhaps
the novelty of the movement explains
why 2 of the 3 Beginners failed to
demonstrate a “normal” gait pattern for
acromio-trochanter segment sway.
Further, based on the analysis of their Abd and ES EMG responses, it appears
the two groups in the current study accomplish
similar kinematic changes with
different, contrasting neuromotor strategies.
This kind of motor equivalence is
a commonly accepted phenomenon
within the motor control literature and
has been noted in the dance literature
as well.[6,12] Understanding that different
individuals might accomplish similar
movements through differing neuromotor
strategies is important to factor
into the use of generalized neuromotor
coaching strategies. Many espouse an
idealized, optimal approach to initiating
and executing a given movement.
However, there may be no such thing as
an “ideal” individual to which an ideal
model applies. For those who are not situated
ideally, optimal performance may
be elicited by very different neuromotor
coaching. “It should…be understood
that no single ideal postural model is
appropriate for all individuals. Instead,
there must be an understanding of the
principles that govern efficient posture.
These principles must then be applied
to each individual.”[19]
The studies by Trepman and associates[13]
and Trepman and coworkers[14]
found variability within and between
subjects during dance movements. The
current study supports these findings.
Because of this within subject variability,
large sample pools with collapsed data
analysis may not be the most informative
approach to a better understanding
of dancers’ strategies in complex tasks.
Individual profiles are lost if only group
analysis is performed. Future research
in this area will most likely benefit from
repeated measures designs that support
ensemble averaging and mixed within
and between subject analysis, so that
a full depiction of the variations of
individual strategies can be examined
alongside group models.
For example, as mentioned earlier,
analysis in the current study proceeded
from individual data to group ensembles.
In general, individual data from the
first day closely matched data from the
second day and two-day ensembles for
individual data matched group ensemble
data quite well. However, two features
worthy of discussion were seen, and in
a third instance, a remarkable difference
between individual and group data was
found.
The two noteworthy features of individual
data as opposed to the group
ensemble data seen in Figure 1 were:
1. A decreased rate of change over
time as seen in the upslope of the Abd
EMG burst at onset and a widening
of the duration of Abd EMG spikes
in ensemble versus individual data for
Experts, and
2. An artificial smoothing of directional
characteristics within ensemble
versus individual kinematic waveforms
for Beginners.
When examining individual Abd
EMG bursts it is probably safe to say
that, in general, each Expert had a greater
rate of increase during the upslope of
Abd EMG spikes at onset and each
Expert had Abd EMG spikes that took
a shorter period of time overall than the
Experts’ ensemble group graphic represents.
Minor variations between Experts
operated to blunt the group ensemble
rate of increase over time for the upslope
of their spikes and, in addition, widened
the group ensemble duration of spikes
when compared to each Expert’s individual
data. Even though this increased
upslope and duration of the Experts’
ensemble data are blunted by the group
statistic, the increased rate of upslope at
onset of Abd EMG and the shortened
time course of Abd EMG bursts can still
be observed for Expert versus Beginner
group graphics for both R and L Abd
EMG data in Figure 1. In other words,
even though these features are blunted
for the Experts in the group model, these
differences in the Experts’ ensemble
group model are still fairly dramatic
in contrast to the Beginner’s ensemble
group model.
Seen on an individual basis, the
Beginners’ Abd EMG’s were erratic,
sometimes bursting unpredictably
throughout the movement with no
apparent coordination between R and
L Abd EMGs and with repeated spikes
distributed broadly through time,
suggesting that their EMG responses
did not have a stable relationship with
major sway characteristics such as sway
reversals. By contrast, the Experts’ Abd
EMG responses were highly stereotypical,
tightly focused, and reliably coordinated
with sway characteristics of the
movement.
The ensemble AP sway data in Figure
1 is represented by what appears to be
a smooth, continuous line indicating
that once a direction for sway had been
established, it was consistent. However,
when individual graphs for AP sway
are examined, Beginners’ sway patterns
are not as smooth as the Experts’. The
Beginners have multiple “movement
units” within their patterns, as well as
greater deviations across trials. This
concept of “movement units” has been
adapted from von Hofsten and Ronnqvist[21]
who define a movement unit
as a directional shift in the slope of the
waveform. A clear reversal of the slope
was required to count as a movement
unit (in other words, if the amplitude
came to a plateau before re-establishing
the preceding slope, it did not count
as a movement unit). The Beginners
in this study demonstrated multiple
movement units within a given trial. By
contrast, the Experts had virtually no
extraneous movement units. Once the
Experts established a direction of sway,
their movement proceeded smoothly
through that direction into a transition
to either the next phase of their overall
sway pattern or to movement resolution.
Some of these movement unit
characteristics can be seen in Figure 1 in
the spread of the lines representing the
standard deviations from the ensemble
means. This spread is greater for the
Beginners and reflects the variability
generated by multiple movement units.
These outcomes are in agreement with
Spriggs and colleagues[22] who measured
jerk (i.e., rate of change of acceleration
patterns) and found that beginning,
advanced, and expert dancers demonstrated
increasing levels of smoothness,
or decreased jerk, during performance
of a dance movement. Differences in
performance variability is a commonly
reported finding in longitudinal studies
of learning that document progressive
skill acquisition and in cross-sectional
studies of differences between novice
and skilled performers.
The third and probably the most
significant difference between the
individual and the group data is that
the kinematic pattern fabricated by
ensemble averaging for the Beginners
does not represent the data of two of the
Beginners. Dufek and associates[23] demonstrated that statistically constructed
models of human kinematic data do
not necessarily represent individuals in
the group. To understand the motor
strategies of individuals, single subject
or within subject designs and analysis
are required. Results of the current
study provide examples of this statistical
phenomenon. For example, all three of
the Beginners’ AP sway graphs are displayed
in Figure 2. The overall shape of
the waveform through time for the top
graph, for Beginner 1, is similar to the
ensemble graphs for each of the Experts
and for the group ensemble graph for
the Beginners. The middle graph, for
Beginner 2, differs from the Experts and
the Beginners’ group ensemble graph in
that the primary direction of the sway
is anterior, without a reversal that goes
posterior further than the baseline at the
start. Beginner 2 tilts forward during the
step and then releases that forward tilt to
arrive at resolution. In other words she
does not oscillate forward and backward
of her starting axis like all of the Experts
and Beginner 1 does and like the Beginners’
group ensemble graph indicates.
The bottom graph in Figure 2, for Beginner
3, does not have any resemblance
to the anterior-posterior-anterior sway
seen for the Experts, Beginner 1, and
the Beginners group ensemble graph.
Instead, it consists of an initial sway in
the posterior direction followed by an
anterior sway to resolution. Again, like
with Beginner 2, there is no oscillation
around her starting axis, but in this
instance, the sway is entirely posterior
to the starting axis, just the opposite of
Beginner 2’s variation.
Being the converse of Beginner 2’s
AP sway, these two data sets explain
how ensemble averaging of Beginner 2
and 3 creates the anterior-posterior-anterior
sway pattern seen for the Experts
and Beginner 1. Beginner 2 contributes
the anterior sway at the beginning of
the step, and Beginner 3 contributes
the posterior sway prior to movement
resolution. The diversions from the
group model seen in Beginner 2 and 3’s
data bring up an important issue that
needs to be addressed in group analysis
of inherently variable phenomena, that
is, individual patterns need to be assessed
along side group modeling in order to
fully understand the data.
In an elegant and stunning work,
Dufek and coworkers[23] performed both
group and single subject analysis on
the same data set to assess the effects of
movement experience on impact forces
during jumping and running. They concluded
that “The group models were not
representative of any of the individual
subjects’ performances and indicated
that group models can describe a mythical
‘average’ performer who in fact is
not representative of any of the actual
performers.”[23] They recommend that
researchers interested in the performance
of individuals carefully evaluate experimental
design before automatically using
traditional group evaluation procedures.
Keppel[24] clearly demonstrates how within-subjects designs control for individual
variability. He goes on to say that in
addition to an increase in the efficiency
of data collection and analysis resulting
from use of within-subject versus group
designs, within-subject designs have
become the designs of choice in studies
of learning and transfer.
In summary, the Expert dancers in
this study exhibited a smooth, dynamic,
and stereotypic movement strategy that
appeared to incorporate anticipatory
responses during forward stepping to
the balance requirement at resolution
of the movement task. In contrast, the
Beginners demonstrated a strategy that
was jerky, had high variability from trial
to trial, showed between-Beginner differences,
and involved apparently compensatory
responses to threats to balance
during and at resolution of the forward
stepping movement task. The Expert
dancers were closer than the beginners
to vertical alignment before, throughout,
and at resolution of this movement task.
Electromyographic responses suggest
that the Experts optimized the work of
core stability with efficient, well timed
bursts of core muscle activity that
helped preserve the acromio-trochanter
segment’s relationship to vertical.
Surprisingly however, while the Experts’
sway oscillated around a more vertical
axis than the Beginners’, the Experts
had the same absolute range of sway as
the Beginners. This finding matches
nicely with a dynamic conceptualization
of ongoing postural configurations
during movement that are normal and
appropriate for forward stepping.
Applications to Teaching and Learning
The results of this study suggest applications
to dance pedagogy, with a focus on
teaching movement through space. By
examining and interpreting the strategies
of the expert dancers, we may be
able to enhance the process of achieving
reliably coordinated, smooth, dynamic
torso movement off the standing base,
with core support, during shift of weight
in traveling work. The results of this investigation
point out the inherent sway
and ongoing dynamic adjustments to
posture during forward stepping. This
adaptable concept of dynamic stability
stands in high contrast to neuromotor
patterns that stiffen against torso sway
in maintenance of vertical posture. If
the smooth, dynamically responsive and
reliable performance model displayed by
the expert dancers in this study is the
desired model, concepts such as holding
or tightening to maintain vertical,
and approaches that encourage restricting adaptive sway of the torso, may be
counterproductive to the coordination
patterns seen in this study and to optimal
weight shift strategies.
How might dance educators assist
dancers in developing the coordination
of smooth, dynamic torso movement
during shift of weight in traveling work?
In an exercise designed to encourage the
strategy seen in this study, a teacher could
assist in the following way. During the
pre-movement “ready” phase, prior to
initiation, take a moment to help the
dancers visualize the action they are
about to perform, talk them through
it using vivid detail that includes the
ongoing changes needed as the demands
of the movement progress. Then, with
these intentions in mind, perform the
action. By promoting this pre-movement
anticipation and intentionality, the
teacher can guide the dancer toward the
discovery and use of neuromotor strategies
like those observed in the expert
dancers in this study.
Finally, teachers can recognize the
need to encourage and support individualized
work. We need to define and
describe the goal of the action and then
allow each dancer to explore and experiment
with multiple strategies until the
most effective one for that individual
is realized. Attention to the task, using
visualization and awareness of initiation
mechanisms, may allow the system to
self-organize, resulting in a reduction
of unnecessary tension and increased
efficiency in muscle use. For decades the
practitioners of somatic practices, such
as Feldenkrais and Alexander, have been
approaching neuromotor re-education
from this perspective of awareness, selfdiscovery,
and ease without restrictive
holding to achieve movement goals. The
expert dancers in this study achieved
the smooth, coordinated shift of weight
with a motor strategy allowing sway and
ongoing dynamic adjustments. Dance
educators might be more effective in
moving dancers toward more appropriate
and efficient strategies such as those
displayed by the expert dancers in this
study by encouraging introspective
exploration of movement, similar to the
way guided experiences are structured in
somatic work, and allowing each dancer’s
neuromotor system to find increasingly
more fluid and elegant patterns
of coordination as options to their old
patterns.
References
2. Blasis C: An Elementary Treatise upon the
Theory and Practice of the Art of Dancing.
Translated by M. S. Evans. New York:
Kamin Dance Publishers, 1953.
3. Kirstein L, Stuart M: The Classic Ballet:
Basic Technique and Terminology. New
York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1976.
4. Lawson J: Classic Ballet: Common Faults
in Young Dancers and Their Training.
New York: Theatre Arts Books, 1973.
5. Barnes MA, Krasnow D, Tupling SJ,
Thomas M: Knee rotation in classical
dancers during the grand plié. Med
Probl Perform Art. 2000;15:140-7.
6. Chatfield SJ, Barr S, Sveistrup H,
Woollacott MH: Electromyographic
and kinematic analysis of movement
repatterning in dance. Impulse,
the International Journal of Dance
Science, Medicine, and Education.
1996;4(3):220-34.
7. Gamboian N, Chatfield SJ, Woollacott
MH, Klug GA: Effect of dance
technique training and somatic training
on pelvic tilt and lumbar lordosis alignment
during quiet stance and dynamic
dance movement. J Dance Med Sci.
1999;3(1):5-14.
8. Krasnow DH, Chatfield SJ, Barr S, et
al: Imagery and conditioning practices
for dancers. Dance Research Journal.
1997;29(1):43-64.
9. Meglin J, Woollacott M: The neural
choreography underlying a pirouettearabesque.
Kinesiology and Medicine
for Dance. 1992;14:95-105.
10. Monasterio RA: Postural adjustment
for voluntary leg movement in dancers.
Unpublished master’s thesis, University
of Oregon, 1994.
11. Mouchnino L, Aurenty R, Massion J,
Pedotti A: Coordination between equilibrium
and head-trunk orientation
during leg movement: a new strategy
built up by training. J Neurophysiol.
1992;67(6):1587-98.
12. Ryman R, Ranney D: A preliminary
investigation of two variations of the
grand battement devant. Dance Research
Journal. 1978/79;11:2-11.
13. Trepman E, Gellman RE, Micheli
LJ, De Luca CJ: Electromyographic
analysis of grand-plié in ballet and
modern dancers. Med Sci Sports Exerc.
1998;30(12):1708-20.
14. Trepman E, Gellman RE, Solomon
R, et al: Electromyographic analysis of
standing posture and demi-plié in ballet
and modern dancers. Med Sci Sports
Exerc.1994;26(6):771-82.
15. Wilmerding V, Heyward V, King M,
et al: Electromyographical comparison
of the developpe devant at barre and
centre. Presented at the 9th Annual
Meeting of the International Association
for Dance Medicine & Science,
Tring, England, 1999.
16. Snijders CJ, Ribbers MTLM, de
Bakker HV, et al: EMG recordings
of abdominal and back muscles in
various standing postures: Validation
of a biomechanical model on sacroiliac
joint stability. J Electromyogr Kinesiol.
1998;8:205-14.
17. Horak F, Nashner L: Central programming
of postural movements:
adaptation to altered support-surface
configurations. J Neurohysiol.
1986;55:1369-81.
18. Krasnow DH, Monasterio RA, Chatfield
SJ: Emerging concepts of posture
and alignment. Med Probl Perform Art.
2001;16:8–16.
19. Lutgens K, Hamilton N: The standing
posture. In: Kinesiology: Scientific Basis
of Human Motion (9th ed). Dubuque,
IA: Brown & Benchmark. 1997, pp.
445-459.
20. Winter DA: The Biomechanics and
Motor Control of Human Gait: Normal,
Elderly, and Pathological (2nd ed). Waterloo,
Ontario, Canada: University of
Waterloo Press, 1991.
21. Von Hofsten C, Ronnqvist L: The
structuring of neonatal arm movements.
Child Dev. 1993;64:1046-57.
22. Spriggs J, Bronner S, Brownstein B,
Ojofeitimi S: Smoothness during a
multi-joint movement: 2D and 3D
analysis between groups of differing
skill levels. Presented at the 12th Annual
Meeting of the International
Association for Dance Medicine &
Science, New York, USA, 2002.
23. Dufek JS, Bates BT, Stergiou N, James
CR: Interactive effects between group
and single-subject response patterns.
Hum Mov Sci. 1995;14:301-23.
24. Keppel G: Design and Analysis: A
Researcher’s Handbook (3rd ed). Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1991.